Aphasia has been in the focus of
neurolinguistics, psycholinguistics, sociolinguistics and a great number of
other fields of research in the past decades. It is a term used for linguistic
disorders caused by damage in a specific area of the brain where language is
primarily affected (Crystal, 1980, p. 140). The vast range of research provides
valuable information about the role of linguistic and communicative functions
in the brain. Moreover, due to the enormous amount of studies new intervention
techniques are emerging in order to help patients of dementia (Crystal, 1980,
p. 149). Nevertheless, as a result of the remarkably divergent outcomes of
different research programs, aphasiology is still a rather controversial field
of study (Crystal, 1980, p. 142). The aim of this paper is to give insight to
aphasiology by providing general information about the definition,
classification and key research issues; in addition, about the importance of
assessment and intervention.
Regarding
the definition of the term aphasia,
two main approaches can be distinguished: the broad definition encompasses
every kind of difficulty with the use of the linguistic functions of the brain,
whether it is formulation, expression or association, along with the damage of
other cognitive functions; for example, the damage of memory and attention.
Although language is in the center, the affected behavioral patterns are
considered to be a part of aphasia as well. The other approach is the specified
approach: it defines aphasia merely in the field of linguistic pathology (Crystal,
1980, pp. 140-141).
Communication disorder caused by
brain damage and characterized by complete or partial impairment of language
comprehension, formulation and use…excludes disorders associated with primary
sensory deficits, general mental deterioration, or psychiatric disorders (Crystal,
1980, p. 141).
The specified approach allows a solely linguistic analysis
with the use of acknowledged categories of language: syntax, morphology,
semantics, phonology and so on. Due to the wide range of results in the
fieldwork of researchers, it is hard to tell which approach is better. It might
be for the best to take linguistic impairments as the primary focus of study but
not excluding disturbances of other cognitive functions (Crystal, 1980, pp.
141-142).
Concerning
the classification of aphasia, two main classification systems can be
identified. The traditional categorization is based on the neuroanatomical site
of the injury that caused the dementia. This model of language pathology
differentiates between the places of injury and the movement of the nerve
signals. Regarding the place of injury, researchers identify Broca’s aphasia (which is in the
anterior part of the left side front vertical brain tissue) and Wernicke’s aphasia (which is in the
posterior part of the left side front vertical brain tissue). With reference to
the movement of the nerve signals, the traditional medical classification
differentiates between motor aphasia
and sensor aphasia (Cummings, 2008, p. 344). This is in close relation to the
Broca’s and Wernicke’s differentiation, because in the case of the former, the
disturbance is in the motor cortex, which is the primary area for all voluntary
muscular movement; in the case of the latter, the injury is in the sensory
cortex, which is primarily responsible for processing sensations from the body
to the brain (Scovel, 2008, pp. 74-75). However, with the recognition of the
importance of linguistic features, the traditional classification has been
replaced with a new system that uses binary features to create broad categories
of aphasia. The categories are the following: nonfluent-fluent, expressive-receptive, executive-evaluative,
production-comprehension, motor-sensory and Broca’s-Wernicke’s. The broad system takes into consideration both
the neurological and the linguistic point of view. It functions more as a
scale, because rarely can a patient be categorized into one clear-cut end of
the binary system. It is common to conclude that a patient is ‘predominantly
something’, although according to a holistic view, aphasia is a mixed disorder (Crystal,
1980, pp. 142-143). Nevertheless, there are specific syndromes that can be
identified in a small number of patients. Examples are pure word deafness and pure
word blindness (Crystal, 1980, p. 148). In addition to the classification
systems, there is one major diagnostic test that incorporates both the
language-based and lesion-based model, which is the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
Examination (Cummings, 2008, p. 345).
Regarding the
fields of research, some aspects are paid special attention by researchers. First
of all, concerning all four linguistic modalities – speaking, comprehension,
reading, and writing –, the extent to which these are damaged is of main importance
(Crystal, 1980, p. 146). There is a major distinction between complete loss of
language abilities and partial loss, the former being called aphasia and the latter being called dysphasia. Although the complete loss is
extremely rare, aphasia is the
generally acknowledged term (Crystal, 1980, p. 141). Moreover, another issue is
the extent to which the range of language use is affected. In different cases
of aphasia different language use is typical; for example, the use of
automatic, fixed phrases or serial sequences. Furthermore, the extent to which
the notion of aphasia is applicable to children and adults is also of main
concern of aphasiology (Crystal, 1980, p. 147). Surgical evidence from
hemispherectomy proves that due to the neuroplasticity of the cerebra, children
under ten years old can develop language in the right side of their brain;
therefore, complete recovery from dissolution of language can be achieved.
Nonetheless, the former finding does not exclude the possibility of dementia in
children. The term developmental
aphasia/dysphasia is used when children do not develop language ideally,
although it is a question whether its use is appropriate, seeing that place
aphasia means language loss (Crystal,
2008, p. 148).
With
reference to the assessment and the intervention of aphasia, great developments
have taken place in the past decades. Due to the widely different nature of the
language disorder, not only narrow linguistic impairments but also
communicative troubles are examined. Although the traditional assessment mainly
focused on linguistic deficits, by now a wide range of techniques has been
developed to facilitate the diagnosis of aphasic patients. It is generally
agreed that early diagnosis is an essential basis of later intervention, seeing
that the treatment of the aphasic disorders focuses on specific linguistic
impairments and put a great emphasis on the communicative effectiveness;
therefore, the use of group therapy and conversational partners has emerged (Cummings,
2008, pp. 349-356).
In
conclusion, the aim of this paper was to introduce the general aspects of
aphasiology: the definition, the classification, the main research issues,
assessment and intervention. It is clear that aphasiology is a lot more complex
field of study and it is yet an intensively developing area of research. Despite
the large amount of fieldwork, there is still a great need for longitudinal
studies. Nonetheless, research in aphasiology proved to be undeniably important
for clinical as well as for theoretical reasons (Crystal, 1980, p. 149). The
generally evolving study of dementia has a positive effect both in neurological
studies, in linguistic pathology and in linguistic therapy.
References
Crystal, D. (1980). Introduction
to language pathology. London: Edward Arnold.
Cummings, L. (2008). Clinical
Linguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Scovel, T. (2008). Psycholinguistics. H. G. Widdowson (Ed.). USA: Oxford University Press.
Scovel, T. (2008). Psycholinguistics. H. G. Widdowson (Ed.). USA: Oxford University Press.
No comments:
Post a Comment