Constant sociolinguistic change in
children’s communication
In my end-term essay I would like
to elaborate on the different aspects and influencing factors of the constant
sociolinguistic change that might be observed in the communication of children
according to several aspects within different environments. By considering the
studies I have found most useful and representative when focusing on
sociolinguistic features observed among children, I would like to offer a
detailed insight through different aspects to their constantly changing use of
language systems within various social frames.
One important aspect that should be accounted is the difference and
change influenced by different locations, and therefore, different social
environment. In a study observing groups of children from inner-cities and from
the suburbs, Entwisle (1968) points out that regarding language and cognitive
development, cultural differences are essential topics to consider. Entwisle
(1968) also introduces her observations, such as when starting elementary
school children from the city centre have more developed language skills at the
measured fields than children from suburban areas. As Hymes states, only
restricted amount of data is available about the personal and community beliefs
and values influencing language use, especially in the case of language
acquisition by children. According to basic language skills, this study is
aimed to measure the differentiation in terms of “sentence length, use of
various grammatical constructions, vocabulary size” etc. the results show that –
among other detailed outcomes of the survey – as the participants grow older –
especially in the first couple of years in school – several capacities,
regarding stimulus and response seem to increase. Meanwhile, it is also
important to mention that depending on the consistence of I.Q., linguistic differences
associated with social class often fade away. However, when focusing on
language behaviour and general cognitive style, there might be a couple of
distinguishable observations that are hardly explainable with the association
between their socioeconomic status and their intellectual level.
Furthermore, I found it also important to consider that this
considerable change was triggered by the event of school-start. In other words,
children stepped out of their environment, and in order to be able to enhance
the success of communication, they needed to acquire further language skills.
Secondly, I have found essential to explore how children communicate in
peer groups, what kind of a hierarchical order they develop and how they learn to
handle conflicts. Corsaro’s (1979) study has shown that in the fields of peer
activities in sociological terms, regarding children’s knowledge about status
and roles, their abilities are less developed and sophisticated from what is
expected. Thus their communication often leads to conflicts and breakdowns. According
to studies he had relied on, children are seemingly aware of the key concepts
of status and role expectations. This can be explained with the frequency of
interactions with adults. Children may even adapt the status of the adults and
transmit it into their role plays. However, the situation seems to be more
complex than that, when taking the linguistic regard into consideration;
seemingly, children’s developing ideas of the conceptions of status and role,
furthermore, their developing linguistic skills may heavily influence the
social positions they take in an interaction.
In general, children proved to produce a relatively wide scale of
linguistic features; however, when it comes to role plays, they tend to rely on
simple linguistic structures.
From this study we can clearly see that children develop a certain kind
of order of roles when it comes to communication in peer groups. Moreover, we
can also observe that the development and the direction of change in their
communication heavily depend on the patterns they acquire from adults.
In a third study we can take a method, called ‘controlled improvisation’
into account. This study turned out to be limitedly successful which lead us to
the observation that children are either insecure or unwilling to cooperate in
an artificial situation. Andersen (1993) analyzed the use of speech forms
(e.g. statements, questions,
and imperatives) in their function as speech acts (representatives, directives,
expressives) but she considers the results to be too complex to be
summarized briefly. This study shows
clearly the developing ability of children to play different roles; this method
is called “controlled improvisation.” However, only an inconsiderable amount of
reliable data can be observed by a method that persuades children into
inconvenient situations. In such situations children tend to either refuse to
cooperate or they act resistantly. Although, in order to conclude this study,
it still counts as a great step in the development of observing the sociolinguistic
skills of children.
As another aspect that should be taken into account when looking at the
development of children’s communication from a sociolinguistic point of view, Kyratzis
(2004) writes about the way how children are constantly shaping cultural and
social categories and introduces a view over language as a “social action that
helps shape reality.” She mentions the main approaches how these cultural
categories are established, such as creation of various games and codes, the
way they elaborate to handle conflicts, the establishment of group-identities,
and the way they overrule already existing cultural conventions through peer
talk.
She defines peer cultures as “stable set of activities or routines,
artifacts, values, and concerns” and establishes them as an outgrowth of the
interactions among peer groups. These peer cultures seemingly include various
concerns regarding gender identity, romantic relationships and relations to one
another within the society and adult culture.
She also considers rituals and games developed by peer groups essential
when understanding their social systems. She also thinks that the role of
language use also has an important role when children organize their games and
rituals, such as creating bonds by involving each other in a hierarchical order
or using different narrative voices.
When solving a conflict, they use many advantageous ways, although
resolution is not their primary objective. She also focuses on the
communicative competence in conflict talk; through the multiplicity of voices
they can animate several figures. According to a study concerning aspects of
Italian discussione, children at times also use features, such as
“interruption devices, predisagreements, emphasis markers, and format tying (Kyratzis,
2004).” Children also have several ways
to evoke sympathy and therefore support from others; furthermore, grammatical
features and the way they cope with adult culture also mirror their
communicative competence.
From this study we can clearly observe peer groups’ communication in
various aspects. We can see that peer communication is constantly changing and
developing, while the use of grammatical features is increasing and becoming
even more varied.
As Llamas and Stockwell (2010) say that when different age-groups are
taken into consideration, the language use of each group tends to vary. In case
of the comparison of the given aspect that are being considered, as time
passes, changes in the use of language can be discovered. This observation can
also point towards the change of language use in history. They call this the ‘apparent
time hypothesis’; a revelation that makes it possible to observe change in
progress.
Llamas and Stockwell (2010) also point out that the ways how individuals
of a community relate to each other should be essentially observed in order to
understand to what extent they are influenced from a sociolinguistic point of
view (regarding the strength of their bonds within the social group) and “how
linguistic features are maintained, reinforced and spread.”
As a conclusion, we could clearly see in the above mentioned examples
and studies that the developmental changes in children’s sociolinguistic levels
are quite varied and depend on many layers of different information. We could
see that location can be an influencing factor, and it is especially observable
when children are placed into a different social environment. Nonetheless, they
seem to integrate quickly in a sociolinguistic level. We could also have a look
at their development of social groups within their peer groups and at their
conceptions of status and role. The influential effect of adult patterns can
also be discovered. Yet in another study
we could see that children are hardly cooperative when being pushed into
inconvenient situations. Then we could observe how children shape their own
environments with the help of developing linguistic skills. Lastly we could
have a look at the revelation how the progress of language change can be
observed in a given timeframe. There are further influencing factors, such as
the time that actually passes and strength of social bonds meanwhile. Although
these several levels of these studies cannot be compared to each other, it is
clearly observable that children constantly acquire several sociolinguistic
attributes that they are capable of using in practice as well observed from the
discussed aspects and with the effect of the influencing factors.
References
Andersen
E. S. (1993). Speaking with Style: The
Sociolinguistic Skills of Children. Language in Society.
Corsaro,
W. A. (1979). Young Children's Conception
of Status and Role. Sociology of
Education.
Entwisle,
D. R. (1968). Developmental
Sociolinguistics: Inner-City Children. American Journal of Sociology.
Kyratzis,
A. (2004). Talk and Interaction among
Children and the Co-Construction of Peer Groups and Peer Culture. Annual
Review of Anthropology.
Llamas,
C., & Stockwell, P. (2010). Sociolinguistics.
In N. Schmitt (Ed.), An introduction to
applied linguistics. London ,
United Kingdom :
Hodder Education.
No comments:
Post a Comment